Jota Melgar wrote: <<<<< Looking at you water parameters and comparing them to Romer's results you= should have gotten close to 50:50 ratio. Why you didn't get it? I couldn'= t tell you, just like I couldn't tell you if the "hard water" Peruvians are= subject to Romer's results. Unfortunately his experiments did not go beyo= nd a pH of 6.5 but if you compare the results he got at 26 C and 29 C at pH 5.5 you see an increase in the number of males from 62.7% to 83%. If you look at a 26 C and pH 6.5, his results show a 43.3%. Now, it is safe to s= ay that at 26 C and pH 7.0 the ratios would have been a little lower but, wh= at if he would have raised the temperature to 29 C (closer to what we found = A. cacatuoides at). Maybe the ratio could've been closer to 50:50. =>>>>>> I think that in order to understand how to interpret the data from Romer's study, it may be helpful to also clarify some statistical concepts. I do not want to complicate the issue just to sound important, but rather to help understanding what to expect on the basis of a given study, and avoid frustrations to hobbyists who get somewhat different results, and wonder if they are doing anything wrong (when instead they aren't). Point 1: Say that a scientist like Romer performs a study on the sex ratio of Apistos in relationship to pH and temperature. He or she will use a given number of pairs to replicate a given number of spawns for each specific water condition, from which the average of the sex ratio for each will be calculated. Given perfect study conditions, this average will be very realistic if it is based on an adequately large number of pairs for each pH and temperature condition, because each individual spawn will have a different sex ratio, but all together, when averaged, they will provide a good GENERAL description of the situation. The mean will in other words summarize the variability of the individual spawn observations. Point 2: Now, say that a week later Mr. Scientist receives a donation of another large batch of pairs of apistos, he/she has free time, and it is decided to duplicate the experiment. Well, this time not only as before each individual spawn (replicated in exactly the same condition) will have a sex ratio different from each other, but also their mean will not be absolutely identical the one previously found. It will be in fact different, on the basis of the number of pairs used in this second experiment, and on the basis of the intrinsic variability of the phenomenon being investigated. This is due to the fact that there is also a variability in the sample mean. The morale of the story is that when we hear: "in such conditions I have seen such and such sex ratio", it is not necessarily true that if we duplicate the same conditions, we will see the same sex ratio. Especially if we obtain only one or two spawns, it will be actually very unlikely (still possible though, but not frequent) that we will get exactly the same. This is way there are indeed statistical procedures that allow the scientist to say (instead of "in such conditions you should see such and such sex ratio"), "if you duplicate my conditions you have a very high probability to observe a sex ratio comprised between ....(low end of the ratio) and .... (upper limit of the ratio)", which gives a much more realistic presentation of what to expect. Depending on how large the original study is, and the natural variability of the event, these intervals of probability can be very narrow (say, between 1:1 and 1:1.2) or very large (say, expect between 5:1 and 1:30). I do not have Romer's paper (is there anyone available to fax or mail it to me, if it is in English? E-mail me, thanks), but unless the study was extremely large, it is unlikely that the estimates calculated allow a very precise forecast. They may however provide a useful guidance of the general rules and trends of the gender ratio determination, which is still an extremely important discovery. I hope to have helped to prevent bad feelings..... Dionigi Maladorno